To compare the effect of three different feedback modalities on swimming pace, sixteen male swimmers and triathletes participated in this study. Each participant swam 3 x 400 m, one for each feedback modality, swimming front crawl at 80% of their individual swimming critical speed. Three feedback modalities were examined: self-pacing, real time visual feedback and real time voice feedback. The swimmers adopted a fast start in all feedback modalities. In the real time voice feedback modality, the data recorded during the second lap (200 m) showed a significant improvement of their swimming pace approaching the swimming pace intended (-1.47 s, p < .01, medium effect size 0.79). A significant improvement toward the swimming pace intended was also noticed at the third split time (300 m) (0.05 s, p < .01, large effect size 0.81) and at the fourth split time (400 m) (0.46 s, p < .01, medium effect size 0.76). In self-pacing, the swimmers were not able to swim in line with the swimming pace intended. In real time visual feedback modality, the swimmers did not show a significant improvement approaching the swimming pace intended. The results revealed that communication with the swimmers using the real time voice feedback induced a significant improvement in their swimming pace and could help the athletes to swim with accurate and consistent pace.
Key words
- training
- performance
- auditory pathways
- sensory
- split time
Nowadays, science and new technology strongly influence many sports. Over the past 30 years, a substantial amount of research has contributed to improving swimming science and its application in training is becoming indispensable. Despite aquatic conditions, the technological progress has allowed the development of some informative instruments that assist swimmers during and after performing a task (Perez et al., 2009). Most of these instruments do not allow real time communication between the coach and the swimmer and some of them need to have the registered data processed after the training session. Real time feedback is important in swimming because the “feeling” about your own performance is usually inaccurate. It is a known fact that the swimmer’s personal feeling of her/his own swimming pace does not always coincide with the external evaluation. Therefore, in swimming, new instruments should allow real time communication between the coach and the swimmer, and at the same time they must be wearable and should not interfere with swimmer’s performance (Bächlin and Tröster, 2012; Turner et al., 2008). It is speculated that the regulation of exercise pace is dictated by a combination of the ability to resist fatigue, feedback integration, anticipatory forecasting and indeed previous experience (Tucker and Noakes, 2009). However, the responsible mechanisms are currently unclear and it remains a controversial issue what constitutes well-executed work pace. Observation of changes in the velocity pattern during competitions has developed an increasing interest in pacing strategy during athletic competitions (de Koning et al., 2011; Mytton et al., 2015). Pacing strategy can be defined as the goal directed distribution and management of effort across the duration of an exercise bout (Edwards and Polman, 2012). The effect of pacing on performance has been studied in many cyclic sports, including swimming (Thompson et al., 2004), cycling (Abbiss et al., 2013), running (Bath et al., 2012) and triathlon (Le Meur et al., 2009; Wu et al., 2015).
Swimming pace can be defined as the speed at which a swimmer completes a distance and indeed is an important variable to be monitored to obtain a better performance (Scruton et al., 2015; Thompson, 2014). The sport of swimming is biomechanically inefficient with high drag force, therefore, appropriate swimming pace is crucial to avoid any undesirable onset of sudden fatigue, which will determine loss of stroke power, coordination and speed (Mauger et al., 2012; Thompson, 2014). It has been reported that the ability to repeat constant swimming pace in different conditions is a quality of competitive and elite swimmers (Skorski et al., 2013, 2014). There is no widespread acceptance of any pacing method for swimming. Traditionally, coaches manage the swimming pace by giving visible coded signals to swimmers with their hands, while standing on the poolside. Some studies have reported that the modalities of feedback given to the swimmer could affect swimming pace (Perez et al., 2009; Turner et al., 2008). Few studies have attempted to use audio pacing signals to communicate swimming pace to the swimmer (Thompson et al., 2002, 2004). We did not find any study where some instruction was given by an instrument that allowed the coach to speak in real time to the swimmer. Moreover, we did not find any study where real time voice feedback was compared to other feedback modalities on swimming pace.
The aim of this study was to compare swimming pace under three different feedback conditions: no feedback or self-pacing, with real time visual feedback or with real time voice feedback. We hypothesized that real time voice feedback would be an effective way to communicate with the swimmer.
Sixteen males participated voluntarily in the study. We selected different levels of experienced swimmers and triathletes with a minimum of two years of sport experience, including 5 swimmers and triathletes that competed in national championships (age 19.70 ± 2.17 years old; body height 176.50 ± 5.99 cm; body mass 70.18 ± 4.82 kg). We chose participants with heterogeneous experience and did not select only swimmers or triathletes of elite national level, because well-executed swimming pace has a huge impact at all levels of swimming and triathlon training and competitions. In this study swimmers and triathletes are both called swimmers.
Each swimmer performed 3 x 400 m, one for each feedback modality, using a front crawl style in a 25 m swimming pool at the swimming pace intended. The swimming pace intended was 80% of their individual swimming critical speed (CSS), which is within an aerobic range (Touretski, 1993). The start of every trial was given with wall push off. Prior to the study the CSS of each swimmer was determined according to the protocol described by Ginn (1993). CSS can be defined as the highest sustainable work rate which enables lactate to remain at a steady-state (Ginn, 1993). CSS is an inexpensive and noninvasive method commonly used to easily evaluate and adjust training pace (Dekerle et al., 2002). The study participants swam with three feedback conditions: self-pacing, real time visual feedback and real time voice feedback with 1 week intervals between the three trials. The swimmer was informed about the swimming pace intended before starting swimming, and further feedback was given every 100 m. Before each trial athletes swam between 1000 and 1500 m for a warm-up consisting of free continuous mixed swim at moderate intensity. The swimmers repeated the same in-water warm-up before each trial. Between the warm-up and the trial, the swimmer was given at least 5-10 min of complete rest.
Swimmers were requested to refrain from any high intensity exercise at least 24 hours before the trials. They were also asked not to consume food for the 2 hours before the trials and abstain from alcohol, caffeine or any other energy drink on the day of each trial. All the swimmers had reported no hearing or visual issues in the 12 months prior to the beginning of the study.
All the modalities were provided to the swimmers over the three trials, but only one of the three modalities was given on a trial. The data was recorded at the beginning of training after the warm-up. There was no familiarization trial because self-pacing and real time visual feedback modalities are used daily during training. The swimmers were informed about their split time during the trials. During every trial, except self-pacing, swimmers were informed about their swimming pace every 100 m, specifically, the difference in seconds with respect to the swimming pace intended was communicated to the swimmers. In order to evaluate the effects of the three feedback modalities on swimming pace, in every trial a split time every 100 m and a total time were recorded. One swim coach experienced in timekeeping was the time keeper for each swimmer. Timekeeping was recorded by a stopwatch 3X - 100 (Finis, Livermore, CA, USA). The real time feedback was provided every 100 m, between 5 – 10 m after the swimmer made the turn. The real time visual feedback consisted of a code of signals-gestures with one's hands, which were explained to the swimmer before the trial session and normally used by the coaches on the poolside. In this study the real time voice feedback consisted of a short voice message, which was given to the swimmer via the Ucoach device (S.H.O.T. Swimmers Hit On Technology, Rome, Italy). This device is a swim training tool designed to talk to the swimmer during a training session providing real time voice feedback. The system consists of a poolside transmitter and a waterproof receiver, the first one allows the coach to communicate in real time, by a small microphone to minimize ambient sound. The waterproof receiver easily fits under any kind of a swimming cap and the voice of the coach is perceived by the swimmer from the headset via moving bone conduction technology. All swimmers were informed verbally and in writing about the nature of the study, including all potential risks. The swimmers signed a written informed consent form prior to the beginning of the study and the ethics committee of the University of Alicante granted ethical approval, according to the Declaration of Helsinki.
Every 100 m difference in seconds between the pre-set speed and the one obtained under the three modalities of feedback was recorded. A difference of zero seconds indicated perfect adaptation to the speed desired, a negative value meant faster pace and a positive value meant slower pace. Standard error and confidence intervals were calculated. In all three tests, the first 100 m were swum without providing information about athletes’ pace. In order to distinguish between the feedback modalities during the 200, 300 and 400 subsequent m, the differences with the swimming pace intended were compared at all times. In addition, each feedback modality was analyzed to determine which feedback was most efficient in helping the swimmers in matching the pace. Standard deviation (SD) of the difference regarding the swimming pace intended could be considered as the measure of accuracy of each feedback modality. Therefore, the differences among the SDs at 200, 300 and 400 m between the three feedback modalities were calculated. We evaluated the effect size as a fraction of the variability between participants. We classified effect size as trivial (0-0.19) small (0.2-0.49), medium (0.5-0.79) and large (0.8 and greater) (Cohen, 1992). Effect size was calculated to determine the smallest worthwhile differences in the trials. To compare and analyze the differences between each modality and between the various distances the paired two-sample t-test was carried out. Not being able to guarantee normal statistical distribution, its equivalent, non-parametric Wilcoxon test was performed. To analyze the differences between the SDs we used the F-test of the equality of two variances, taking into account the robustness of the F-test with respect to equality of sample size. The SPSS 15.0 was used to carry out this analysis.
The mean pace difference of the three different feedback modalities, comparing the swimming pace intended, is shown in Table 1. In all the three modalities, the swimmers had no information within the first 100 m of the 400 m effort and there was no significant difference in swimming pace of the first split time, between the three modalities. With regard to the pacing pattern, the swimmers adopted a fast start in all the modalities. In the self-pacing modality and in the real time voice feedback modality, 68.75% of the swimmers adopted a fast start, meanwhile in the real time visual feedback modality it was 87.5%. The results revealed that at 100 m the swimmers swam between 2.87 (real time voice feedback modality) and 4.73 s (real time visual feedback modality) faster than the swimming pace intended (Table 1).
Overall results of the mean differences in seconds from zero (swimming pace intended) throughout the three 400 m trials, as well as the absolute differences among feedback modalities
100m (SD) | 200m (SD) | 300m (SD) | 400m (SD) | 100 vs. 200m | 100 vs 300m | 100 vs. 400m | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
-3.65 (1.13) | -3.21 (1.97) | -2.84 (2.68) | -3.21 (3.27 | ) | 0.44 (0.53) | 0.81 (0.67) | 0.44 (0.79) | ||
(-6.06, -1.24) | (-7.40, 0.98) | (-8.55, 2.87) | (-10.18, 3.75) | 0.12 | 0.12 | 0.05 | |||
-4.73 (1.10) | -4.11 (1.81) | -4.32 (2.07) | -5.08 (2.56) | 0.61 (0.53) | 0.41 (0.88) | 0.36 (0.68) | |||
(-7.06,-2.39) | (-7.98,-0.25) | (-8.73, 0.10) | (-10.53, 0.37) | 0.17 | 0.08 | 0.05 | |||
-2.87 (1.39) | -1.47 (1.36) | 0.05 (1.59) | 0.46 (1.74) | 1.40 (<0.01) | 2.92 (<0.01) | 3.33 (0.01) | |||
(-5.83,-0.09) | (-4.37, 1.41) | (-3.35, 3.45) | (-3.25, 4.18) | 0.79 | 0.81 | 0.76 |
Absolute Differences among feedback modalities | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
100m (Sig.) | ES | 200m (Sig.) | ES | 300m (Sig.) | ES | 400m (Sig.) | ES | ||
1.08 (0.47) | 0.17 | 0.90 (0.71) | 0.13 | 1.48 (0.68) | 0.17 | 1.87 (0.87) | 0.18 | ||
0.78 (0.65) | 0.25 | 1.74 (0.41) | 0.24 | 2.89 (0.39) | 0.27 | 3.67 (0.66) | 0.27 | ||
1.85 (0.44) | 0.37 | 2.64 (0.19) | 0.42 | 4.37 (0.09) | 0.54 | 5.55 (0.12) | 0.52 |
The swimmers in real time voice feedback modality were able to reach swimming pace close to the intended one. In this feedback modality, throughout the 400 m swim, the participants gradually swam closer to the swimming pace intended, whereas in self-pacing and in real time visual feedback such improvement was not revealed (Figure 1). In the real time feedback modalities as soon as the first split time (100 m) was recorded, it was quickly communicated to the swimmer. In the real time voice feedback the data recorded during the second lap (200 m) showed a significant correction of athletes’ swimming pace approaching the swimming pace intended (
Figure 1
Swimming pace in three different feedback modalities The graph shows the mean differences in seconds from zero (swimming pace intended) throughout the three 400 m trials: real time voice feedback (black triangles), real time visual feedback (white circles) and self-pacing (black circles). Student’s t test ** p < .01; non-parametric Wilcoxon test ** p < .01. SP: Self pacing; VIF: real time visual feedback; VOF: real time voice feedback.

In self-pacing, the participants were not able to swim in line with the swimming pace intended, they adopted a fast start and during the 400 m they did not correct their swimming pace (Figure 1, Table 1). In real time visual feedback modality, after the hand signals-gestures at 100 m, the swimmers did not show a significant correction approaching the swimming pace intended at the second split time (200 m), neither in the next split times at 300 m and 400 m (Figure 1, Table 1). Surprisingly, the real time visual feedback was less effective than the rest of modalities in terms of times closest to swimming pace intended (Figure 1), but it was difficult to ensure that swimmers were able to see the visual feedback given during the trial. We also compared the mean difference with swimming pace intended between feedback modalities every 100 m, and at 300 m and 400 m split times we recorded a significant difference between real time voice feedback and real time visual feedback (
In real time voice feedback, medium and large effect size was recorded according to Cohen’s classification (Table 1). Meanwhile trivial effect size was recorded in self-pacing and real time visual feedback (Table 1). Table 2 shows the estimated SD values relative to the pace of each feedback modality. Figure 2 graphically shows an increase in the variability of the SD in self-pacing and real time visual feedback. Self-pacing and real time visual feedback show higher values at 200 m with meaningful differences between 100 m and 200 m (
Figure 2
Standard deviation (SD) of swimming pace in three different modalities of feedback The graph shows the mean values of SD throughout the three 400 m trials: real time voice feedback (black triangles), real time visual feedback (white circles) and self-pacing (black circles). During the trials the SD was more stable in real time voice feedback, as compared to self-pacing and real time visual feedback. The graph shows significant difference between self-pacing and real time voice feedback at 300 m and 400 m. F-test *p < .05, ** p < .01; SP: Self pacing; VIF: real time visual feedback; VOF: real time voice feedback.

Overall results of standard deviation throughout the three 400 m trials.
SD 100m | SD 200m | SD 300m | SD 400m | 100 200vs. m | 200 300vs. m | 300 400vs. m | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
4.52 | 7.86 | 10.72 | 13.07 | 3.02 (0.02) | 1.86 (0.12) | 1.49 (0.23) | ||
4.39 | 7.25 | 8.28 | 10.23 | 2.73 (0.03) | 1.30 (0.31) | 1.53 (0.21) | ||
5.55 | 5.42 | 6.38 | 6.98 | 0.95 (0.54) | 1.39 (0.27) | 1.20 (0.37) |
F Snedecor among feedback modalities | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
F. 100m (Sig.) | F. 200m (Sig.) | F. 300m (Sig.) | F. 400m (Sig.) | |||||
1.06 (0.46) | 1.18 (0.38) | 1.68 (0.16) | 1.63 (0.18) | |||||
1.51 (0.22) | 2.10 (0.08) | 2.82 (0.03) | 3.51 (0.01) | |||||
1.60 (0.19) | 1.79 (0.14) | 1.68 (0.16) | 2.15 (0.08) |
Instead, with real time voice feedback, SD values did not show any substantial changes during the whole trial (Figure 2), even if with this feedback modality the initial value at 100 m was highest when compared with the other two modalities. Furthermore, the differences between SD values of the real time voice feedback and of self-pacing at 300 m (
All the swimmers reported an additional motivational boost with the real time voice feedback modality, when they heard the voice of the speaker via the Ucoach device. We do not have any objective measurements about the drag forces produced by the Ucoach receiver, but all participants declared that the receiver was quite comfortable to wear. The subjective feedback, given by the swimmers, suggests that the Ucoach receiver did not compromise swimming performance with any additional water resistance or by disturbing fluid dynamics.
Swimming pace has an impact on performance and there are some studies that have researched the distribution of the split times during pacing (de Koning et al., 2011; Skorski et al., 2013, 2014; Wu et al., 2015). This is the first study that has evaluated the effects of the real time voice feedback modality on swimming pace. Previous research studied audible bleep feedback (Thompson et al., 2002, 2004), but not human voice feedback. Also, the obtained data was compared with other feedback modalities, and this ensured additional relevance to the results.
In swimming, the modalities of self-pacing and real time visual feedback are used during training with different distances and different pace. Daily use of these modalities during training could exclude or minimize any learning effect. Our results showed that it was not an easy task for non-elite swimmers to control their own pace by themselves. Previous studies have shown that elite swimmers are able to reproduce accurate swimming pace (Skorski et al., 2014), however, this does not appear to be as precise as a coach might expect (Thompson et al., 2002). Many swimmers are not elite athletes at the top of the Fédération International de Natation (FINA) or International Triathlon Union (ITU) ranking points and our results could have a wide impact on the swimming world including youth athletes. The swimmers adopted a fast start and this finding reinforces the statement that in swimming the “feeling” about your own performance is usually inaccurate; in addition, our results about a fast start are in accordance with previous studies (Mauger et al., 2012; Skorski et al., 2013, 2014). Nonetheless, it is still unclear why the swimmers adopted a fast start.
In any sport performance, control of pacing seems to be a combination of intrinsic (Latash, 1998) and extrinsic feedback (Swinnen, 1990). In swimming, extrinsic feedback factors could play a crucial role in regulating eventual changes if there are self-misperceptions. A previous study has shown that different feedback conditions could affect the control of swimming speed (Perez et al., 2009). Real time feedback is an input of great value for training, it allows immediate feedback and this is more effective than delayed feedback (Grosser and Neumaier, 1986).
Previous studies have demonstrated that different types of feedback can improve swimming pace control and other swimming variables (Bächlin and Tröster, 2012; Hagem et al., 2013a, b; Perez et al., 2009). The audio feedback modality has been shown to be useful in swimming (Thompson et al., 2002; Turner et al., 2008). Our results revealed that the real time voice feedback modality, via the Ucoach device, greatly helped the swimmers in controlling the swimming pace. Data showed that the swimmers maintained consistent pace when the real time voice feedback was given. We know that the auditory system is preferential in the marine mammal group (Ketten, 2002); based on our results and previous studies on audio feedback (Thompson et al., 2002, 2004), the auditory system could be a preferred way to communicate with swimmers in the aquatic environment. The obtained data might be considered important for the development of a training workout in the aerobic range where the real time voice feedback could be very useful. Further research should be conducted with faster swimming pace close to competition pace. Some recorded differences may not only have statistical significance, but they could be especially interesting for coaches. Our sample was heterogeneous, thus further research with real time voice feedback is necessary in elite and inexperienced swimmers. Moreover, future studies should be performed also with the other three swimming styles (backstroke, breaststroke and butterfly).
SD values were stable during the trials and showed that the real time voice feedback, given via the Ucoach device, was the most accurate feedback modality among the three ones used in this study. We strongly believe that Ucoach is a useful device for swimming pace training, as well as any other tool that allows speaking with swimmers during in-water training.
In this study, communication with swimmers was regarding their swimming pace, without giving any other additional information. However, it is thought that using the Ucoach device, the coach could give many other short messages to swimmers regarding other aspects of performance, such as technique, body posture, body rotation in crawl and backstroke, stroke efficiency, leg kicks, feedback during turns and rhythm or breathing cycle changes and any other aspects considered important for coaches and swimmers. Real time voice feedback via the Ucoach device could also be useful when swimmers swim in open water, which is a more difficult training environment. The real time voice feedback could be used in other aquatic sports such as synchronized swimming and lifesaving sport.
This study demonstrates that real time voice feedback could be an accurate modality for helping swimmers in maintaining their swimming pace. In this regard, the swimmers had a good response after real time voice feedback and they reached stable pacing as desired. Compared with other modalities, the real time voice feedback could be the best way to manage swimming pace. The real time voice feedback seems to be an effective way to communicate changes in swimming pace during training. Swimmers became immediately familiar with the Ucoach device and it appeared that the communication provided via the auditory system allowed an accurate and reliable modality for controlling swimming pace. The study shows that real time voice feedback by the Ucoach device made the communication with the swimmers effective and easy, and this tool has wide applications in swimming, not only in swimming pace. Moreover, with the real time voice feedback the coach could motivate and encourage the swimmers for a greater commitment and provide them with continuous technical corrections from the poolside.
Figure 1

Figure 2

Overall results of standard deviation throughout the three 400 m trials.
SD 100m | SD 200m | SD 300m | SD 400m | 100 200vs. m | 200 300vs. m | 300 400vs. m | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
4.52 | 7.86 | 10.72 | 13.07 | 3.02 (0.02) | 1.86 (0.12) | 1.49 (0.23) | ||
4.39 | 7.25 | 8.28 | 10.23 | 2.73 (0.03) | 1.30 (0.31) | 1.53 (0.21) | ||
5.55 | 5.42 | 6.38 | 6.98 | 0.95 (0.54) | 1.39 (0.27) | 1.20 (0.37) |
Overall results of the mean differences in seconds from zero (swimming pace intended) throughout the three 400 m trials, as well as the absolute differences among feedback modalities
100m (SD) | 200m (SD) | 300m (SD) | 400m (SD) | 100 vs. 200m | 100 vs 300m | 100 vs. 400m | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
-3.65 (1.13) | -3.21 (1.97) | -2.84 (2.68) | -3.21 (3.27 | ) | 0.44 (0.53) | 0.81 (0.67) | 0.44 (0.79) | ||
(-6.06, -1.24) | (-7.40, 0.98) | (-8.55, 2.87) | (-10.18, 3.75) | 0.12 | 0.12 | 0.05 | |||
-4.73 (1.10) | -4.11 (1.81) | -4.32 (2.07) | -5.08 (2.56) | 0.61 (0.53) | 0.41 (0.88) | 0.36 (0.68) | |||
(-7.06,-2.39) | (-7.98,-0.25) | (-8.73, 0.10) | (-10.53, 0.37) | 0.17 | 0.08 | 0.05 | |||
-2.87 (1.39) | -1.47 (1.36) | 0.05 (1.59) | 0.46 (1.74) | 1.40 (<0.01) | 2.92 (<0.01) | 3.33 (0.01) | |||
(-5.83,-0.09) | (-4.37, 1.41) | (-3.35, 3.45) | (-3.25, 4.18) | 0.79 | 0.81 | 0.76 |
Inter-Limb Muscle Property Differences in Junior Tennis Players The Pre-Exhaustion Method Does Not Increase Muscle Activity in Target Muscle During Strength Training in Untrained Individuals Neuromuscular Impact of Acute Hypertrophic Resistance Loading With and Without Blood-Flow Restriction Validity and Reliability of the Smart Groin Trainer for Measuring Hip Adduction Strength The Repeated Curve Sprint Test Appears to be an Appropriate Tool for Estimating Anaerobic Fitness in Young Trained Male Futsal Players Training with a Heavy Puck Elicits a Higher Increase of Shooting Speed Than Unloaded Training in Midget Ice Hockey Players Accuracy of Predicting One-Repetition Maximum from Submaximal Velocity in the Barbell Back Squat and Bench Press The Importance of Posture And Body Composition for the Stability and Selected Motor Abilities of Professional Handball Players Influence of Tactical Behaviour on Running Performance in the Three Most Successful Soccer Teams During the Competitive Season of the Spanish First Division Post-Activation Performance Enhancement in Sprinters: Effects of Hard Versus Sand Surfaces Cognitive Factors in Elite Handball: Do Players’ Positions Determine their Cognitive Processes? Effects of Postactivation Performance Enhancement on the Vertical Jump in High-Level Volleyball Athletes On-Court Change of Direction Test: An Effective Approach to Assess COD Performance in Badminton Players Movement Patterns of Polish National Paralympic Team Wheelchair Fencers with Regard to Muscle Activity and Co-Activation Time The Creation of Goal-Scoring Opportunities at the 2019 FIFA Women’s World Cup Assessing the Sprint Force-Velocity Profile in International Football Players with Cerebral Palsy: Validity, Reliability and Sport Class’ Profiles Handcycling Training in Men with Spinal Cord Injury Increases Tolerance to High Intensity Exercise Hand Grip Strength vs. Locomotor Efficiency in Sitting Volleyball Players Evidence‐Based Recovery in Soccer – Low‐Effort Approaches for Practitioners Anaerobic Variables as Specific Determinants of Functional Classification in Wheelchair Basketball Addition of in‐Play Cooling Breaks During Intermittent Exercise while Wearing Lacrosse Uniforms in the Heat Attenuates Increases in Rectal Temperature The Effects of Preferred Music and Its Timing on Performance, Pacing, and Psychophysiological Responses During the 6‐min Test Prevalence of Dehydration and the Relationship with Fluid Intake and Self‐Assessment of Hydration Status in Czech First League Soccer Players A Study Comparing Gait and Lower Limb Muscle Activity During Aquatic Treadmill Running With Different Water Depth and Land Treadmill Running Changes in Heart Rate Variability and Post‐Exercise Blood Pressure from Manipulating Rest Intervals Between Sets of Resistance Training